[Pollinator] Fwd: FW: CA Beekeeper's' rebuttal to Harvard Study
Ladadams at aol.com
Ladadams at aol.com
Tue Apr 10 12:13:03 PDT 2012
____________________________________
From: gludwig at almondboard.com
To: Ladadams at aol.com
Sent: 4/9/2012 5:30:36 P.M. Pacific Daylight Time
Subj: FW: CA Beekeeper's' rebuttal to Harvard Study
Hi Laurie,
As an FYI, you may want to attach this along with the just sent out notice
re the study from Harvard.
Gabriele
Sent: Monday, April 09, 2012 1:03 PM
Subject: CA Beekeeper's' rebuttal to Harvard Study
Randy Oliver, a beekeeper in CA, was one of many who posted a rebuttal to
the Harvard paper on imidacloprid and bees. "My reading of the paper
suggests that the author knows little about bees, little about pesticides,
nothing about HFCS, had no understanding of the distribution of systemic
pesticides in plants. This paper is an example of authors so bent on 'proving'
that imidacloprid is the cause of CCD, that they strain credulity with some of
their assumptions and reasoning, and even by changing the experimental
protocol midstream!"
To read the entire article, please see below.
----------------------------------------------------------------------------
----
News Items
The Harvard Study on imidacloprid and CCD
April 2012
Re this study, at first glance it appears to support the hypothesis that
chronic exposure to field realistic doses of imidacloprid during summer and
fall can lead to late winter collapse of the treated colonies.
The study got off to a good start—several colonies were fed different “
field realistic” doses of imidacloprid in syrup, and colony populations and
brood area were measured. If the authors would have stuck to this original
design (which has already been performed numerous times in several
countries) the results would have been meaningful. Indeed, after a month of
feeding such syrup, the investigators did not observe any adverse effects upon
the colonies due to the insecticide!
But then, since the lead investigator seemed to be eager to “prove” that
CCD is caused by imidacloprid, he dreamed up the fantastic scenario that in
the winter of 2006/2007 that for some inexplicable reason the nation’s
supply of HFCS was contaminated with high levels of imidacloprid. My reading
of the paper suggests that the author knows little about bees, little about
pesticides, nothing about HFCS, had no understanding of the distribution
of systemic pesticides in plants. This paper is an example of authors so
bent on “proving” that imidacloprid is the cause of CCD, that they strain
credulity with some of their assumptions and reasoning , and even by changing
the experimental protocol midstream!
When the investigators failed to prove their case after a month of feeding
spiked syrup—they changed the protocol, and ramped up the doses of
insecticide in the syrup to sky high and acutely toxic levels, and then made a
series of compounding mistakes, notably by not performing the sort of
necessary parasite management required for colonies to survive the winter. And
then, the symptoms of the colonies when they died did not match the symptoms
of CCD, yet the Harvard press agent claimed that they did!
Unfortunately, there are also a great number of factual misrepresentations
and quite a bit of fuzzy thinking in the paper, which obviously was not
peer reviewed by any bee biologist nor toxicologist. I realize, in
retrospect, that some of my comments may sound a bit snarky, and I apologize to the
authors, whom I’m sure were earnest in their quest to prove their
anti-neonic agenda. Back to the paper, allow me to discuss some of the problems.
The author stated in an interview:
“When other conditions cause hive collapse—such as disease or pests—many
dead bees are typically found inside and outside the affected hives.”
Could someone please refresh my memory? Other than in the case of
tracheal mite, which diseases or pests leave many dead bees in a hive? (Note that
starvation or acute pesticide toxicity would not fall into the category of “
disease or pest”). The point is, that the natural behavior of sick or old
bees is to abandon the hive—one normally does not find dead bees in hives
that have died from parasites, including viruses.
Let’s look at a few more sentences from the paper:
“We hypothesized that the first occurrence of CCD in 2006/2007 resulted
from the presence of imidacloprid … in high-fructose corn syrup (HFCS), fed
to honey bees as an alternative to sucrose-based food. There are three facts
to support this hypothesis. First, since most of the suspected but
creditable causes for CCD were not new to apiculture, there must have been an
additional new stressor introduced to honey bee hives contemporaneous with the
first occurrence of CCD during the winter months of 2006 and early 2007.”
“their beekeeping practices have been relatively unchanged during these
years except for the replacement of honey or sucrose with HFCS as the
supplemental sugar source for economic and convenient reasons…. Although the
replacement of honey/sucrose-based feeds with HFCS among commercial beekeepers
took place much earlier than 2006/2007, it was the timing of the
introduction of neonicotinoid insecticides to the cornseed treatment program first
occurring in 2004/2005 that coincides with CCD emergence.”
The authors give no justification for their assumption that there was any
change in HFCS in 2006. And as Bob Harrison and others have pointed out,
CCD actually started occurring in 2004-2005, prior to the authors’
assumption that tainted syrup hit the market beginning in 2006. Any HFCS produced
from such treated corn would have necessarily have been produced following
the season of harvest.
”Second, while commercial beekeepers appear to be affected by CCD at a
disproportional rate…”
This is simply not true according to any of the several surveys that I’ve
seen (see papers by CCD researcher Dennis van Engelsdorp). Indeed
beekeepers who have never fed HFCS experienced plenty of cases of CCD.
The authors then cite a few studies that show that systemic insecticides
are translocated, as they are intended, throughout the plants. But then
they stretch by stating:
“ These study results lend credence to our hypothesis that the systemic
property of imidacloprid is capable of being translocated from treated seeds
to the whole plant, including corn kernels and therefore likely into HFCS.”
My gosh, this is one helluva assumption! Without taking the time to
simply confirm that imidacloprid winds up in the kernels, the authors assume
that it is concentrated there at high levels! And then they further go out on
a limb by assuming that any such imidacloprid is then somehow concentrated
when the corn is used to produce HFCS (ignoring the fact that most corn is
treated with clothianidin, rather than imidacloprid):
The paper turns into farce when the author states:
“we used food-grade HFCS fortified with different levels of imidacloprid,
mimicking the levels that are assumed to have been present in the older
HFCS.”
Why in the world would the authors “assume” that imidacloprid was present
in the older HFCS, but not present in the HFCS that he used in the current
study to feed the control colonies? But then they go on to state:
“ The range of dosages used in this study from 20 to 400 ìg/kg were not
only environmentally relevant…”
Since when has 400 ppb ever been been considered to be “environmentally
relevant”? Levels of 1-4 ppb are environmentally relevant; levels above 40
ppb are usually considered to be overtly toxic. So the 400 ppb figure is 100
– 400 times as strong as the normal measured levels in the field due to
seed treatment.
As if that weren’t enough, the authors go into la-la land with some even
wilder creative assumptions:
“Since there is no tolerance level for imidacloprid in HFCS, we applied a
10-fold concentrating factor, or 0.5 ppm (500 ìg/kg) of imidacloprid in
HFCS, by taking into account the uptake by corn plants from seeds that are
treated with imidacloprid.”
They simply created this “concentrating factor” out of thin air! They
give absolutely no justification for it. In the actual process of making
HFCS, pesticides are largely removed. As I stated before, all that the authors
had to do would have been to ask Roger Simonds at the USDA Gastonia
pesticide testing lab as to the actual measured levels of imidacloprid in HFCS,
and thus would not have brought embarrassment to Harvard School of Public
Health by such a ludicrous assumption.
“Therefore, we are confident that the imidacloprid dosages applied in this
study would be comparable, if not lower to those encountered by honey bees
inside and outside of their hives.”
Unfortunately, the authors’ confidence is not supported by any actual
field measurements whatsoever!
The authors state: “There are several questions that remain unanswered as
a result of this study. First, the systematic loss of sealed brood in the
imidacloprid-treated and control hives may indicate a common stress factor
that was present across all 4 apiaries.”
Like, maybe the field investigators should have taken a few nosema or
varroa counts, rather than simply assuming that these common parasites weren’t
killing the colonies! For all we know, all the hives could have bee
crawling with varroa or badly infected with nosema. One statement suggests that
varroa was evident: “nor a large number of Varroa mites was observed in
hives during the summer and fall seasons,” which suggests to me that the
investigators are admitting that some something less than a “large” number of
mites was indeed observed!
Let’s look at varroa: the study states that 3-lb packages were installed
on March 28. Surprisingly, “By May 21st, 2010 all twenty frames in each
of 20 hives were drawn out into comb and contained at least 14 frames of
capped brood.” These colonies really took off, meaning that they were virtual
varroa breeding grounds. By late July they averaged about 25,000 cells of
sealed brood.
Strange and Calderone (2009) found Eastern package bees to contain about 3
mites per hundred bees, which would work out to about 300 mites in a 3-lb
package. When colonies are rapidly expanding, mite populations double each
month. So from late March through late July, we’d expect the mite
populations in these hives to reach 4,800 by late July. This is a very serious
mite infestation level! Yet, the researchers waited until October 5 to treat
with Apistan strips (which are ineffective against mites in many areas of
the U.S.)! Any experienced beekeeper would suggest that these colonies
died from a varroa/Deformed Wing Virus epidemic, which leaves deadouts, as the
authors observed, “remarkably empty except for stores of food and some
pollen left on the frames.” Unfortunately, the authors only included a photo
of a honey frame, rather than a brood frame, which might have been helpful
in diagnosing the actual cause of death! The dosing with high levels of an
insecticide would be expected to cause the treated colonies to suffer more
from varroa than the untreated controls.
The description of the dead colonies does not match the definitive signs
of CCD at all—there was a dwindling of population, rather than a sudden
collapse, and no abandoned brood. Rather the descriptions of the deadouts more
closely matched dwindling collapse due to varroa/virus or nosema.
The authors, on a roll, simply do not know when to stop: “If imidacloprid
exposure is truly the sole cause of CCD, it might also explain the scenario
in which CCD occurred in honey bee hives not fed with HFCS. Considering
the sensitivity of honey bees to imidacloprid as demonstrated in this study
and the widespread uses of imidacloprid and other neonicotinoid
insecticides, pollen, nectar, and guttation drops produced from those plants would have
contained sufficient amounts of neonicotinoid insecticide residues to
induce CCD.”
What are they talking about when they say “considering the sensitivity”?
Even the lowest fed dosage (20 ppb) is about 5-20 times higher than that
commonly found in nectar, and the other three doses were far higher–it is
amazing to me that the colonies were not killed outright!
Speaking of which, I find it odd that the investigators didn’t give any
explanation as to why they changed treatment dosages mid trial. To their
credit, they initially treated the colonies with “field realistic” doses of
the insecticide: 0.1 – 10 ppb. I suspect that after feeding the colonies
for four straight weeks in July, and not noticing any adverse effects, that
they then decided that they had better really hit the colonies hard if they
wanted to “prove their case”–so they arbitrarily ramped up the lowest
dose to 200 times stronger, and the highest dose to 40x stronger (that oughtta
do it!).
I can only imagine their surprise and disappointment when after nine
weekly feedings of a full half gallon of syrup intentionally spiked to widely
accepted toxic levels, that they still noted virtually no adverse effects!
Surprisingly, the amount of broodrearing was unaffected at the 20, 40, and
200 ppb dosages, and only slightly depressed at the clearly toxic 400 ppb
dose! Note that the colonies were all still alive at midwinter, 3 months
after the dosing ended. If anything, this study clearly demonstrated that
colonies of bees can survive prolonged poisoning by imidacloprid at
excessively high levels!
So why did the colonies die? Such insecticide exposure to hives in late
summer has been previously demonstrated to greatly increase the chance of a
colony later dying from nosema or varroa infection during the winter. In
this study, poisoning the colonies all through late summer and early fall
likely hampered the ability of the colonies to prepare a healthy population
for winter.
Oddly, the investigators also took biweekly measurements of the cluster
sizes of the colonies, yet chose not to include the results in the paper.
This makes me wonder whether the authors simply decided to exclude any data
that did not support their hypothesis.
So although this paper is surely going to be cited by anti-neonic
advocates as some sort of supportive evidence, I find it to be a case in which an
initially well-designed study (the dosing of hives with a series of four
field realistic doses of imidacloprid) turned to farce when the investigators
arbitrarily ramped up the doses, and blew it on parasite management.
In my assessment, it appears that the data from this study actually
support an alternative hypothesis–that field realistic doses of imidacloprid had
no measurable adverse effects upon the colonies. And even patently toxic
doses had little immediate effect. I suspect that the apparent delayed
effect was due to the impact of the insecticide upon late summer colony
populations (which the authors inexplicably did not present), which led to later
collapse due to parasite buildup.
In reality, the neonicotinoids fully appear to be “reduced risk”
insecticides, which under field conditions, when properly applied (no dust issues)
have never been associated with significant colony health issues. Compared
to alternative insecticides, the data to date (including that of this
study) support the hypothesis that neonicotinoids are an improvement over the
previous classes of insecticides (there are clearly some questions about
dust issues, chemigation, foliar and landscape treatments, which I will
discuss in an upcoming article).
I find it unfortunate that the press, including both of our national bee
journals, gave publicity to this paper without any sort of critical
analysis. Such messages only confuse the public. Pesticides are a major issue to
the beekeeping community. What we need are well designed and executed
studies, (as well as better enforcement of pesticide law) in order to solve
these problems. Sadly, this study just confuses the issues.
_http://scientificbeekeeping.com/_ (http://scientificbeekeeping.com/)
____________________________________
____________________________________
=
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: <http://lists.sonic.net/pipermail/pollinator/attachments/20120410/a62cb3d6/attachment.html>
More information about the Pollinator
mailing list